GOOD-LITE 270400 Contrast Sensitivity Test User Guide
- June 5, 2024
- GOOD-LITE
Table of Contents
- [](https://manuals.plus/wp-content/uploads/2022/07/GOOD-LITE-logo.png
- Introduction
- [Test Procedure When Using Low Contrast Visual Acuity Charts
- LEA Low Contrast Gratings
- The Types of Contrast Sensitivity Changes
- Testing Procedure
- References
- Read User Manual Online (PDF format)
- Download This Manual (PDF format)
[](https://manuals.plus/wp-content/uploads/2022/07/GOOD-LITE-logo.png
)GOOD-LITE 270400 Contrast Sensitivity Test
LEA Contrast Sensitivity Test
Introduction
Measurement of contrast sensitivity has been used in experimental and clinical
research for several decades, in some places also in routine examinations of
patients with different vision problems.
The clinical tests that have been developed by LEA-Test have been useful in
the evaluation of vision of thousands of patients revealing changes in visual
function undetectable with the usual high contrast visual acuity tests.
Because low contrast vision is not yet covered in all teaching programs, some
basic issues, like the definition of contrast and recommendations on
luminance, are included in this text.
Since contrast sensitivity tests have not been routinely used, the values
measured with the different tests may not depict the quality of visual
function when one starts to use the tests. The same was true when high
contrast visual acuity tests began to be used.
The combination of high and low contrast visual acuity values de-fines the
location of the slope of the contrast sensitivity curve but diagnostically the
most important feature is any change over time.
We have been taught that 1.0 (20/20, 6/6) is “normal” visual acu-ity although
actually it is at the lower end of the range of normal values between 0.8
(20/25, 6/9) and 2.5 (20/8, 6/2.5). Similarly, a rather common low contrast
visual acuity value is 0.5 (20/40, 6/12) at 2.5% contrast when a person has
normal sight. However, the variation of visual acuity at low contrast is
nearly five – six lines, similar to variation at high contrast.
Now it is easy to measure, record and detect changes in the trans-fer of
visual information when the change only affects visual acuity at low contrast
levels. Repeated measurements and observation of changes in visual functioning
will increase our understanding of how the measured values depict visual
impairment. Other visual functions need to be assessed as well for
classification of visual impairment but the simple visual acuity measurements
at different contrast levels give us an easy start.
What is Contrast Sensitivity?
Contrast sensitivity measures the ability to see details at low con-trast
levels. Visual information at low contrast levels is particularly important:
- In communication, since the faint shadows on our faces carry the visual information related to facial expressions;
- In orientation and mobility, where we need to see such critical low-contrast forms as the curb, faint shadows, and stairs when walking down. In traffic, the demanding situations are at low contrast levels, for example, seeing in the dusk, rain, fog, snowfall, and at night;
- In every day tasks, where there are numerous visual tasks at low contrast, like cutting an onion on a light colored surface, pouring coffee into a dark mug, checking the quality of ironing, etc.;
- In near vision tasks like reading and writing, if the information is at low contrast as in poor quality copies or in a fancy, barely readable invitation, etc.
Contrast sensitivity is the reciprocal of the contrast at threshold, i.e., one
divided by the lowest contrast at which forms or lines can be recognized.
If a person can see details at very low contrast, his or her contrast
sensitivity is high and vice versa. Depending on the structure of the stimulus
used in the measurement – either gratings of different size or symbols –
contrast sensitivity of a person gets different values.
What is Contrast?
Contrast is created by the difference in luminance, the amount of reflected
light, reflected from two adjacent surfaces. It can be defined in slightly
different ways. In clinical work, we usually use the Michelson formula
:
Lmax = Luminance on the lighter surface
Lmin = Luminance on the darker surface
When the darker surface is black and reflects no light, the ratio is 1. Contrast is usually expressed as percent, then the ratio is multiplied by 100. The maximum contrast is thus 100% contrast. The symbols of the visual acuity charts are close to the maximum contrast. If the lowest contrast perceived is 5%, contrast sensitivity is 100/5=20. If the lowest contrast perceived by a person is 0.6%, contrast sensitivity is 100/0.6=170. There is no international recommendation on how contrast of visual acuity charts should be defined. Therefore there are differ-ences in the contrast of tests of different manufacturers.
Which Luminance Level Should Be Used?
An international recommendation does not exist on the luminance level for
contrast sensitivity testing, but there is a recommendation for visual acuity
testing. It recommends a luminance level equal or higher than 85 candelas per
square meter (cd/m²).
In the United States and in a number of other countries, measure-ment of
visual acuity for research purposes is done by using the back illuminated
ETDRS light box with the luminance level adjust-able from 220 to less than 1
cd/m² by using layers of filters. In the small light box the maximum luminance
level is 125 cd/m².
Measurement of Contrast Sensitivity
Measurement of contrast sensitivity resembles audiometry: a pure tone
audiogram depicts which are the weakest pure tones at different frequencies
that the person can hear. Contrast Sensitiv-ity Curve or visuogram shows the
faintest contrasts perceived by the person. If the stimulus is a sine wave
grating, then the curve depicts similar function as does the pure tone
audiogram. If the stimuli are optotypes (letters, numbers or pediatric
symbols), rec-ognition is required and the test resembles speech audiometry.
As in audiometry, the result of the contrast sensitivity measurement is not
one single value but a diagram.
Figure A
Contrast sensitivity curve
Visual acuity is plotted along the horizontal axis and contrast sensitivity
along the vertical axis (Figure A). The size of the symbols decreases along
the horizontal axis and they become paler and paler in the vertical direction
(Figure B). The boundary between symbols perceived and those that are too
small or too pale and thus not seen, is depicted by a curve, called Contrast
Sensitivity Curve. Its declining right-hand slope is the most interesting part
of the curve in clinical cases. To define the slope of the contrast sen-
sitivity curve, we need two or three measurements. The first one defines the
point at the x-axis, the visual acuity value determined in the usual way. The
second is the definition of the upper end of the straight part of the slope
usually located in the 1-5% contrast area. An additional measurement at lower
contrast is often of interest.
The threshold values can be measured with two different tech-niques when using
optotype tests:
- By using low contrast visual acuity charts, or
- By using tests with one symbol size and several contrast levels.
Test Procedure When Using Low Contrast Visual Acuity Charts[
](https://manuals.plus/wp-content/uploads/2022/07/GOOD-LITE-270400 -Contrast-Sensitivity-Test-fig-4.png)
Testing is identical to the mea-surement of visual acuity at high contrast
level, i.e., we measure the smallest size of the optotypes that the person can
recognize. The threshold is defined as the line on which at least 3 out of the
5 optotypes are correctly recognized. The 2.5% test is the most practical test
in clinical use. The resulting threshold point on the curve is far enough from
the high contrast value so that the declination of the slope of the curve can
be defined. In severe low vision, the test must be quite close, which may
require use of reading lenses.
Move quickly down the chart and ask the person to identify the first or the
last symbol on each line. When the person hesitates or makes an error, recede
one line and ask the person to read the entire line. To record the result
carefully, record the number of optotypes read correctly, i.e., if on the 2.5%
chart one of the sym-bols was read incorrectly on line 20/63 (6/18, 0.3)
record the visual acuity value as 20/63 (-1) at 2.5%.
Contrast Sensitivity Measured By Using Low Contrast
Visual Acuity Charts
Translucent Low contrast visual acuity charts for the small light box. The optotypes are printed at 2.5% (the old 25%, 10%, 5%, and 1.25% contrast charts are still available). Low contrast chart is available also for the ETDRS cabinet.
- Translucent 2.5%Low Contrast Symbols Chart (#253800)
- Translucent 2.5%Low Contrast Numbers Chart (#271600)
Test results are marked on the recording sheet at the level used (see example below) going along that level toward the right until the visual acuity value, measured at that contrast (A’ at 1.2%, B’ at 2.5%), is reached. If the person’s visual acuity was 20/20 (6/6, 1.0), the line connecting these three points, A’, B’ and X, depicts the slope of the contrast sensitivity curve of this person. Record the results as 20/50 (6/15, 0.4) at 2.5% and 20/100 (6/30, 0.2) at 1.2%.
Test Procedure When Using Low Contrast Tests with One Symbol Size
In this test type the 10M size is convenient because at the most common
testing distance of 1 meter, it corresponds to visual acuity 0.1 (20/200,
6/60), at 2 meters to 0.2 (20/100, 6/30), at 4 meters 0.4 (20/50, 6/15) and at
0.3 meter distance 0.03 (20/600, 6/180) covering the low contrast visual
acuity range of most per-sons with visual problems. The contrast levels of the
test lines on the five pages are 25%, 10%, 5%, 2.5% and 1.2%.
Example of using the Low Contrast Test Recording Form
The same diagram that was used when testing with the low contrast visual
acuity charts, can be used also for reporting of the results from testing with
tests with one symbol size. The number of correct answers is read on the left
vertical axis and the cross is placed corresponding to the distance used when
testing. Luminance variation affects the threshold values in many disorders
and even in normally sighted individu- als. It is difficult to arrange high
enough luminance on the test in a regular room except by pointing a light Low
Contrast Flip Charts (#251100) source toward the test. Then the amount of
light falling on the test is dependent on the distance of the test from the
light source. For reliable follow-up measurements, the test should be at a
fixed distance from the light source. In field surveys, the variation in the
results caused by varia-tion in illumination needs to be taken into
consideration.
The surface of the low contrast charts is easily damaged. Avoid touching the
white test surface. If a person needs to point with his/her finger at the
optotype to fixate on it, use tube gauze or a white glove to cover the finger.
- If the person has not seen the contrast tests before, explain that you are using a low contrast test the same four symbols that were used in the high contrast testing (“ the same pictures that we just looked at and you gave the names…”).
- Start measuring at 3 meter distance if you expect close to normal visual functioning.
- Ask, which is the first picture on the line. If the person sees the first number/picture correctly with ease, move to the next page. Continue this way until the person makes an error.
- If the person answers incorrectly, ask, which is the next picture and then next going through the whole line. If the person sees three out of five symbols correctly and does not see any numbers on the next line, mark the result as the sum of all the optotypes on the previous lines + the three on the last line read.
- To make it easier to know how many optotypes there has been on the previous lines, there is a small number on the left lower corner of each page showing the sum of optotypes on the previous pages and the contrast level marked on the right lower corner.
- When coming to a new page, the child may say that “there is nothing” on that page. It is difficult to focus on optotypes close to the threshold values. Therefore, tell the child to look at the optotypes on the previous page and not to move the gaze when the page is turned. Quite often the child can see the whole line that (s)he did not perceive when the image was not in perfect focus on his/her retina.
- For measurement at larger optotype sizes, move closer. If the first measurement was at 3 meters, move to 1.5 meters and measure until threshold.
- The results are marked on the recording form as the crossing of the lines marking the testing distance used and the number of optotypes seen correctly.
The result of the measurement is written down as the number of correct answers. Although only one optotype was tested on each line, each line above the threshold line is credited for 5 correct answers. Thus, if a person reads 3 of the 10M LEA SYMBOLS® correctly on line 5 at a distance of 1.6 meters, the result is written: LEA-S, 10M:23 at 1.6m. The result can be marked on the diagram by locating the test distance on the upper border of the recording form (top scale) and marking the results (in this case 23) at the point where the line of the test distance (1.6m) and the line of the number of the correct answers cross each other. This point is marked with S in the dia-gram. In the diagram, it is easy to calculate that the result can also be written 0.16 (-2) at 1.2% contrast (0.16 = 20/125 or 6/38). The number of correct answers is read on the left vertical axis and the cross is placed corresponding to the distance used when test-ing. In this case the result would then be 18 correct at 3m (marked M) and 23 correct at 1.6m (marked S). Line S-M-X depicts the slope of the contrast sensitivity After having used the test for a while, you will not need the record-ing form anymore, except for reporting your results to somebody who is not accustomed to using the test. You will have a mental image about where the threshold is located on the form. You will mark down the name of the test, the number of correct answers and the distance at which you measured. These three numbers carry the necessary information for follow-up. When both optotype and grating measurements are made, it is interesting to mark them on the recording form to see the relation-ship between the different threshold values. The luminance of the tests needs to be kept closely equal, otherwise the results are not comparable. This contrast sensitivity recording form can be used to record any test results from contrast sensitivity tests. The comparison of test results from different tests is easy when they are plotted on this form. By combining results from optotype tests and grating tests the quality of the central visual field can be evaluated for low vision or occupational assessment.
LEA Low Contrast Gratings
Grating Acuity Test at Low Contrast Levels
Grating tests have been used to measure contrast sensitivity since the 60’s.
These computer-controlled grating tests have not be-come widely used in
clinical medicine because they are expensive and require an experienced
technician to use them. However, studies with them have taught us some
important principles in measurement of contrast sensitivity in cases of low
vision. The most important finding is that contrast sensitivity values in A
nearly all cases of low vision 100 are different when measured with gratings
of different sizes: 40º 10º 5º 2.5º the larger the grating, the 10 Grating
Diameter higher the contrast sensitiv- ity value. This is particularly common
in cases of central 1 scotoma that ‘eats up’ some of the stimulus and thus the
effective stimulus is smaller than the physical
stimulus.
Small grating stimuli would often give a misleading pic-ture of visual
function at low contrast. Therefore it is wise to make one measurement with a
large grating stimulus to learn about the subject’s ability to see low
contrast information. On the other hand, it is interesting to evaluate the
function of the fixation area by using a smaller stimulus. This is possible by
covering the grating stimulus with a grey folder that leaves either one fourth
or one tenth of the stimulus visible.
- Contrast sensitivity as a function of stimulus size in normally sighted sub-jects: the larger the grating, the higher the contrast sensitivity values at low spatial frequencies.
- Contrast sensitivity curves of a person with optic atrophy in both eyes. With the 2.5 degree stimulus contrast sensitivity is barely measurable, yet with the 20 degree stimulus the values at low spatial frequencies are normal.
- Contrast sensitivity curves in case of macular degeneration, L = the normal left eye, R = the right eye with dry macular degeneration. Contrast sensi-tivity measured with 10 degree stimu-lus is nearly as good as in the normal left eye, whereas when measured with 5 degree stimulus it is one fifth of the maximum value of the 10 degree curve and when measured with 2.5 degree stimulus the maximum value is only one twentieth of the 10 degree maximum.
LEA Low Contrast Gratings measure contrast sensitivity at 3 contrast levels, 100%, 10% and 2.5% using 3 grating frequencies: 0.5cpcm, 2cpcm and 8cpcm. The grey folder covers part of the grating and leaves visible either one forth or one tenth of the stimulus. With this simple technique it is possible to find out the effect of stimulus size on the threshold values. When the grating is turned into a new orientation it is covered with a grey surface.
Guide
Start with the high contrast grating. Show the gratings at a distance of 2.5m
starting with the 0.5cpcm grating. Turn the gratings in different orientations
before exposing them from behind the grey cover. Do not move the grating when
pre-senting it. Ask the person to respond by showing the orientation of the
lines with his/her hand or with the ruler that is included in the test. The
threshold value is reached when three out of five presentations lead to
correct response. If the broadest lines could not be seen at 2.5m distance,
move closer until they are seen. Step back a little, turn the grating behind
the cover and present it again. If a person has uncorrected astigmatism,
grating will be seen at different distances when presented in different
orientations. Thus you learn about the person’s refractive error while
measuring grating acuity. If the broadest lines were seen at a distance of
2.3m, grating acuity is 2 cpd (0.5cpcm equals 0.5cpd at 57cm, 1cpd at 114cm
and 2cpd at 230cm). This is a low value. When testing normally sighted persons
one starts by showing the finest grating at about 1.5m distance, moves
backward until the lines cannot be discerned and then moves toward the person
until they become visible again. At this distance make the presentations while
slightly changing the distance in order to find the threshold value. Test
first with the large stimulus and then with the two smaller ones. The distance
needs to be measured accurately. It is easy to measure if the person sits with
his/her head supported on the headrest of a corneal microscope and a tape
measure is fixed on the head rest. When the distance is measured the result
can be read on the corresponding nomogram (Diagram A).
For example, if a person saw the 8cpcm grating at 1.15m distance, grating
acuity is 16cpd with a 10 degree stimulus (the grating is 20cm in diameter,
thus 20 degrees at 57.2cm and 10 degrees at 115cm distance, Diagram B).
Grating acuity at the lower contrast levels is measured similarly to the
measurement at high contrast level. It takes some time to get accustomed to
locating the correct value on the nomogram. It is best to mark down the result
on a diagram immediately. If an error is made in one of the measurements the
Nomograms: Grating acuity (cpd) as a function of testing
distance, A. 0.5cpcm grating, B. 2.0cpcm grating and C. 8.0cpcm grating. D.
Diameter of the stimulus as a func- tion of testing distance.
resulting curve looks strange. Then the strange value should be measured
again. If the approximate multiple values of 57.2 cm are used (115, 170, 230,
285 and 340) grating acuity is easier to calculate. It is 2,3,4,5,or 6 times
the cpcm value of the grating or between two of these values. This helps in
locating the exact value on the nomogram. The result is marked on the answer
sheet the same way as the values measured with optotype tests. Grating
frequencies are on the bottom line.
The Range of Normal Contrast Sensitivity
Among the normally sighted people, both visual acuity and contrast sensitivity
have a wide range of variation. In visual acuity, 20/25 (6/9, 0.8) is a low
normal value; the highest normal values are three times higher, 20/8 (6/2.5,
2.5). Similarly, the range of normal variation in contrast sensitivity values
is great. Therefore, a value within the range of normal may or may not mean
that particular person has normal contrast sensitivity. If his or her con-
trast sensitivity was previously high, it may decrease to less than one-half
or one-third of its original value and still be
“normal.”
A change in contrast sensitivity is the diagnostically important feature that
will be watched in the future. Because of the large varia-tion in the normal
values, we need to have an older value to compare with to notice a change.
Ideally, contrast sensitivity and visual acuity should be measured when
children leave their high school/ secondary school or in young adulthood.
These A change in contrast sen-sitivity is the diagnostically important
feature that will be watched in the future. Because of the large variation in
the normal values, we need to have an older value to compare with to notice a
change. Ideally, contrast sensitivity and visual acuity should be measured
when children leave their high school/ secondary school or in young adulthood
.
Result of measurement at the three contrast levels. The person saw the 8cpcm
grating at 270cm at high contrast, which corresponds 38cpd (F). The 10%
contrast 8cpcm grating was seen at 110cm distance, which corresponds to 15cpd
(G). The 2.5% contrast 2cpcm grating was seen at 174cm distance, which
corresponds to 6cpd (H). The line H-G-F depicts the slope of the contrast
sensitivity curve. It is Type III curve.
values should be recorded and saved as part of the basic information related
to each person’s health. A change warrants an ex-amination to find out the
cause of the change. Although the most common cause would be a small change in
the refractive power of the eye, which is a benign finding, repeating the
measurement of contrast sensitivity would be beneficial as a part of routine
health examinations to rule out changes in the visual
pathways.
Measurement of contrast sensitivity would also help us to better understand
the complaints of a person whose visual acuity at high contrast has not
changed but whose vision has decreased at low contrast levels. Then we would
not annoy him/her by saying that his/her vision is as good as before, a
situation which is now experienced by all too many patients/clients. If
occupational tasks require good visual function at low contrast levels, visual
acuity alone does not select the most suitable per-sons for that particular
task. For example, if the task is to notice airplanes approaching within low
clouds, these planes are best seen by a person with good visual acuity in the
100 contrast range of 1-5%. Since the declination of the slope varies even in
normal 10 individuals, it is possible that a person with lower visual acuity
at high con- trast has better function at the lower contrast levels than a
person who has higher visual acuity at high contrast. This is important to
remember in all such occupational tasks that require exceptionally good
visual function at low contrast levels.
Variation in the declination of the slope in normal subjects.
The Types of Contrast Sensitivity Changes
Usually the loss of visual function is roughly equal at high and at low con-
trast levels. The slope of the curve moves toward the left without a change in
the declination (Type I). When there is a small circumscribed lesion in the
center of the macula, visual acuity may de-crease several lines, yet in the
low contrast vision there is slight or no loss (Type II). Type III change in
the transfer of visual information is charac-terized by moderate to no loss of
visual acuity at high contrast and a greater loss of visual function at low
contrast. This is often caused by diabetic retinopathy, cataract, glaucoma, or
optic neuritis, to mention some of the most com-mon causes.
Clinically, it is well known that there can be three Types of changes in
contrast sensitivity. people with different
types of contrast sensitivity losses even when they have similar visual fields
and visual acuity values. They can have very differ-ent functional vision. The
three people whose contrast sensitiv-ity curves are in figure all have visual
acuity of 20/63 (6/18, 0.3). Person A has high normal function at low
contrasts and functions like a normally sighted person. Person B has somewhat
decreased low contrast function and the typical behavior of a person with low
vision (bringing texts closer and moving slightly slower on stairs, etc).
Person C has lost visual functions at low contrast and is severely visually
impaired. Of these three people with the same visual acuity, one is normally
sighted, one has low vision and one is severely visually
impaired.
Lower Visual Acuity May Mean Better Vision
If occupational tasks require good visual function at low contrast levels,
visual acuity alone does not select the most suitable per-sons for that
particular task. For example, if the task is to notice airplanes approaching
within the low clouds, these planes are best seen by a person with good visual
acuity in the contrast range of 1-5%. Since the declination of the slope
varies even in normal individuals, it is possible that a person with lower
visual acuity at high contrast has better function at the lower contrast
levels than a person who has higher visual acuity at high contrast. This is
important to remember in all such occupational tasks that require
exceptionally good visual function at low contrast levels.
Hiding Heidi Low Contrast Face Test (#253500)
Contrast sensitivity needs to be assessed in children and adult persons who are unable to re-spond verbally or by pointing. If the person can follow a moving target or shift gaze to or turn head to peripherally presented visual stimuli, preferential look-ing test situations can be used when testing with Hiding Heidi pictures. Present the test within the distance within which the person visually responds using the highest contrast (100%) first. If you expect normal function in a baby, you may shorten the test situation by showing next the 2.5% picture and then the 1.2% picture. If you do not get a response to the 2.5% picture, show the 25% or 10% picture next and then the 5% picture. The picture is presented by moving both the picture and the white card with the same speed, usually horizontally. If the person has horizontal nystagmus, the pictures are best presented vertically.
The visibility of facial features can be tested also in older children
by using the Hiding Heidi test. Then it is more fun to ask the child point to
Heidi when she becomes visible. In testing of difficult-to-test children we
have sometimes used the following technique: The test is on a table. One of
the two persons testing takes the picture of Heidi, the other tester takes the
blank card. The testers move to the testing distance and ask “who has the
Heidi card?” Some children like to wave “bye-bye” to Heidi.
Whenever contrast sensitivity has decreased, it is advisable to ensure
visibility of facial features at different distances. Surprises are common.
Since the area of the Heidi picture – and that of a face – is so much larger
than the area of a symbol or even a grating stimulus, the low contrast
pictures may be discernible at unexpectedly long distances. However, it is
important not to force children to function at their threshold. If the
function of a healthy child at the same luminance level is demonstrated, the
teachers and therapists will better understand the requirements of the
visually impaired child’s communication.
The ability to detect objects of low contrast is an important component of the
visual system. Determining the levels of contrast that an infant can detect,
helps planning information for intervention and provides a baseline to
evaluate future changes. Deviations from usual behavior may indicate disorders
that leave vision at high contrast levels unaffected.
Visual communication is the most important way of communicating during the
first year of life. Expressions on faces are mediated by faint shadows and
changes of the contours of the mouth and eyes. Most facial expressions are in
low contrast, so an infant’s reaction to the Hiding Heidi Low Contrast Cards
offers useful information. The cards can also be used with multi-handicapped
people.
If an infant only responds to high contrasts, the people in his or her life
should be aware of this problem and make their faces more vis-ible. This can
be done by wearing lip and eye liners, bright lipstick and eyeglasses with
dark frames.
Testing Procedure
Even though “infant” is referenced in the following procedure, the test can also be used on young children and multi-handicapped people.
-
Stack the Hiding Heidi Low Contrast Cards sequentially with the 2.5%, 10% and 100% faces downward, in that order. Since the 25%, 5% and 1.25% faces are on the opposite side, they will face up.
-
Position the infant so he or she faces the examiner and in the optimal position for best visual performance. Support his or her head so involuntary motor movements least affect the infant’s performance. The infant can look over the parent’s shoulder while being held, sit in their lap or in the child’s buggy. Consider the infant’s most comfortable position. If possible, select the best time of day when the infant is most alert. Note any differences in performance when not taking the above into consideration.
-
Before observation of the infant’s responses to the Hiding Heidi faces, familiarize yourself with the infant’s usual response pattern and look for: the head turning toward an interesting visual object, eye widening, breathing, quieting, eyebrow arching, smiling, babbling to or reaching for an object. This will help detect if there are variations of these patterns as the infant fixates on the Hiding Heidi faces. Familiarize and prepare the infant for locating Hiding Heidi in whatever way is appropriate to his or her level.
-
During your communication with the infant, notice how far you can back away from the infant without losing his or her attention to your face. Record this distance, so you can later document changes in the infant’s visual sphere.
-
Leave the stack of cards within your reach, out of the infant’s sight. When presenting the cards, place them in front of your chest. Present the face cards, one at a time, with the blank card in front of the face card. Encourage the infant to look toward the midline by talking to him or her just above the cards, or play Peek-A-Boo with the blank card in front of your face in an attempt to get the infant’s attention.
-
Use two cards for each presentation. One card is always the blank card, the other, one of the six Hiding Heidi faces. Hide the stimulus card behind the blank card. Then ask the child “Where is Heidi hiding?”, while moving the blank card off to one side and the stimulus card off to the other side. Both cards should leave the midline at the same speed. Stimulus cards should be moved to the right and/or left in a random order. The cards are presented in the following order: 100%, 10%, 2.5% and 1.25%. If the infant does not react to the 10% card, present the 25% card. If the infant then reacts to the 25%card, proceed with the 10% card and lower the contrast cards until a threshold level is reached. If the infant does not react to the 2.5% card, present the 5% and other cards, as above, until a threshold level is reached. If the child responds to the 1.25% face, the contrast threshold at that distance is below 1.25%. Record that as <1.25%. The purpose of this order of presentation is to find the infant’s contrast threshold as quickly and as accurately as possible. Avoid repeated presentation of the same stimulus card, as this causes habituation. The tester may notice that an infant does not follow the movement of the Heidi-picture with eye movements or with combined eye-head movements but makes a quick shift of gaze to the picture when it stops. Another child may follow the movement but looks puzzled when the movement stops and looks at the tester as if asking “Where did the picture disappear to?” These observations need to be reported
to the child’s neurologist because they may mean that the child has problems in motion perception (= perception of movement or perception of objects that stand still). I n the examination of older children the child may prefer waving to Heidi “bye-bye” instead of simply pointing. Also, the presentation may be varied by letting the parents show the cards: They hold the cards behind their back while moving to the testing distance. There they present the Heidi card and the blank card at the same time and ask “Who has the Heidi picture?”. -
If the infant does not respond to the low contrast cards, bring them closer. Note the distance. If the infant still does not respond to a horizontal presentation of the face cards, slide the cards in a vertical presentation.
-
I initially present the cards in usual illumination level (average room lighting). If the infant does not respond, increase or decrease the luminance level by utilizing a lamp with controlled lighting that allows you to vary the luminance level. Record the optimal luminance level for communication repeatedly during the first year of life.
-
Since infants rely on near and far visual communication, try to obtain at least two separate thresholds. First, measure at the near communication distance, using the methods described above; record the distance from the child to the cards, the luminance level, and the threshold contrast level reached.
I f the infant responds to low contrast face stimulus at near distance, use one of the cards with higher contrast and the blank card, backing away from the infant to the distance where he or she lost response to your face. Record this distance, the luminance level and the threshold contrast level reached at this distance. This will demonstrate to the child’s parents/therapist/teacher the distance at which the infant still responds to visual information at low/intermediate contrast levels.
Summary
Assessment of visual function at low contrast adds an important dimension in
the evaluation of a person’s capabilities. It should be a part of the
evaluation of vision in occupational health and in low vision services as well
as in all diagnostic work. With the easy-to-use optotype tests, it is possible
to assess visibility of low contrast details. A person’s ability to see low
contrast lines requires grating tests, which presently are under construction.
References
Read User Manual Online (PDF format)
Read User Manual Online (PDF format) >>